[KB] Evolutionary Confirmation and "Deacon"-struction

Edward C Appel edwardcappel at frontier.com
Tue Mar 31 16:19:52 EDT 2015


Burkophiles,

Attached is a revision and expansion of the summary of points of intersection between Burke and Terrence W. Deacon I posted on 10/21/14.  As I say in the document, a bit of "Deacon"-struction and a few overall reflections will come later.



Ed  
--------------------------------------------
On Tue, 3/24/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com> wrote:

 Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
 To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
 Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org" <kb at kbjournal.org>
 Date: Tuesday, March 24, 2015, 1:38 PM
 
 Burkophiles,
 
     Before I get into summary reflections on
 Deacon/Cashman, symbolism, theology, and dramatism, I think
 I need to add more on D and C’s take on the third synergy,
 the one that begets the particularly “religious”
 emotions.  The reason is, as I read between the lines,
 the dislogistic expressions “idiosyncratic” and “idiot
 savants” ring in the background, for me at least, in the
 authors’ treatment of the emergence of “narrative” and
 the “virtual” symbolic world of “hidden” meanings
 and beliefs that derives from the “thesaurus”
 complex.  These two coalescences lead, as I mentioned,
 or can be seen to lead, up and into the airy realm of
 supernatural Beings, essences, and eternal
 destinations.  If you’re a scientist, you’re in
 trouble going there.  Kenneth Miller at Brown
 University (author of Finding Darwin’s God: A
 Scientist’s Search for Common Ground Between God and
 Evolution) is both a renowned evolutionary biologist
  and practicing Roman Catholic, but you won’t find many in
 that mold.  That’s why Miller is called on so
 frequently to testify against Creationists in states like
 Kansas.  He has impeccable credentials on both sides of
 the debate.  Call it a kind of cross-referencing
 ethos.  Call it the power of dialectical
 identification.
 
     In what we’ve said so far about Deacon
 and Cashman on the emotional synergy, there’s more of an
 implicit respect emanating there, I surmise, owing to a
 somewhat less supramundane trajectory from those discordant
 combinations of the cognitive-cum-emotional features of
 mammals in general.  This globally sublime, more
 universally applicable, sense of emotional emergence comes
 out strongest in the authors’ treatment of the
 distinctively “ethical” dimensions of symbolic life.
        
     Deacon and Cashman suggest more regard
 for the emotional synergies that anatomize these
 “religious/spiritual experiences: awe, reverence, a sense
 of the sacred,” yes, but also “selfless action for
 others, a sense of unity with the cosmos, charity, humility
 and lovingkindness.”  These higher-order,
 ethically-tinged, emotional synergies can characterize
 nontheistic Buddhists, the religiously tolerant humanists I
 commune with at UUCL, the spiritual dimensionality sought by
 the Sam Harrises of the world,* anyone who thinks deeply and
 inclusively about man and woman’s creaturely place and
 social obligations in this unfathomable universe. 
 “Religion” in this sense becomes a common quest for
 inner peace and harmony that goes beyond brand-name
 transcendental dogmas.  I sense Deacon and Cashman’s
 putting more of an imprimatur on such dispositions,
 accessible to all.
 
     Here’s the ethical/emotional injunction
 Deacon and Cashman find most illustrative of the novel,
 contradictory, and emergent nature of distinctively human
 feelings and attitudes: “One of the most extreme cases of
 symbolic synergy in the ethical realm is the religious
 virtue indicated by the request: ‘Love thine
 enemies.  Do good to those who harm you.’ 
 Clearly, this is an injunction that juxtaposes some of the
 most deeply ingrained but mutually incompatible emotional
 predispositions.  What the reductionist explanations of
 religious origins overlook, then, is the authentic human
 value of emergent emotional experiences that are the
 intended effect of certain religious symbols and
 practices.”  Such a radical cognitive-emotional
 synergy “undermine[s] the more mundane and normal
 relationships with the physical world” no nonsymbolic
 mammal would have a clue to.
 
     Maybe a summation yet before I invite you
 to retreat back into your dogmatic slumbers.
 
 
     Ed
 
 
     *”I thought I Hated the New Atheists,
 Then I Read Sam Harris’s New Book,” Trevor Quirk, New
 Republic, September 10, 2014, review of Waking Up: A Guide
 to Spirituality Without Religion. 
 
 --------------------------------------------
 On Fri, 3/20/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
 wrote:
 
  Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
  To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
  Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
 <kb at kbjournal.org>
  Date: Friday, March 20, 2015, 12:45 PM
  
  Burkophiles,
  
      Up to this point, Deacon and Cashman say,
  their analysis of the emergence of religion via symbolism
  can fit at least in part within the “reductionistic”
  put-downs of most previous evolutionary schemas. 
  Religion can be shoehorned into the “spill-over” bin,
 or
  the “by-product” box, in the sense that
 “narrative”
  at its base does not equal religion.  Only the tendency
  toward a “telos” inherent in the narrative mode will
  lead almost inevitably in that direction, at least in
 terms
  of a capacity for conceiving of religious dimensions of
  life.  As for the “metaphysical dualism” theme,
  that scenario begins with the
  dictionary/thesaurus/words-in-their-inherent-and-separate-interconnectedness
  intuition that only secondarily, as it were, ramifies
  perceived underlying connections outward and upward toward
  some ultimately meaningful and explanatory
  “beyond.”  Sounds to a Burkean like the motive of
  perfection at work in each case, the
   dialectical “Upward Way,” the imperious
 negative,
  opening vistas of infinity, eternity, and transcendental
  fulfillment of one kind or another.
  
      The third “synergy” generated by
  symbols cannot be so extenuated, the authors assert. 
  This emergent coalescence of generic mammalian faculties
  that symbols induce results in complex, unprecedented
  emotions drawn from primary ones chimps presumably
  possess.   These emergent emotions include,
  among others, “elation,” “awe,” “equanimity,”
  and “spiritual renewal.”  These human
  transcendences result from no “simple mix.” 
  “Efforts to explain these phenomena as mere
 extrapolations
  from more basic adaptive mechanisms evolved for more
  instrumental purposes” prove unsatisfactory, the authors
  claim.   “Without the linkages made
  possible via symbols, these component [“neuronal”]
  processes would not interact, due to mutually exclusive
  generative contexts.”  Focal dynamic mechanism:
  “The ways symbols can reorganize cognition in
  unprecedented ways.
  ”
      Recall what I said in a previous post:
  Brain evolution in the direction of our species, the
  symbolic species, across the length of the Quaternary, was
 a
  RECIPROCAL process, the emerging symbolic faculty altering
  brain development, as well as brain evolution facilitating
  symbolization.
  
      As they get into the nitty-gritty of this
  dynamical process of complex emotional emergence, D and C
  make reference to more elaborated treatments of the issue,
  including some by Deacon himself, but still soldier on in
  abridged explanation.  Cognitions and accompanying
  emotions are the way of life for symbolizers, as well as
  nonsymbolizers.  Those emotional “arousal states”
  are considerably tamped down, though, or can be so, in
 human
  thought and  experience.  Prefrontal brain
  enlargement, both generating and generated by gradual
  evolutionary symbolic progressions, has conferred on
  symbolizers an “executive” capacity toward more
  pronounced self-control.  It, the symbolically enlarged
  and enhanced brain, has facilitated not only the cognitive
  flexibility to juxtapose and ramify myriad blends of
  thoughts and ideas, but also those of accompanying
 emotions
  or arousal states,  Those cognitions can be harmonized
  or accommodated in their disparity in a
   way that the potentially incompatible emotions
  cannot.  Thus, “symbolic blends can bring together
  otherwise incompatible or opposed component
  emotions.”  To put the matter another way, the
  “interaction” of these “blended emotional states . .
 .
  produces a synergistic effect that cannot be reduced to
 any
  simple additive relationship (i.e., not a simple 40 %
 angry
  60 % surprised composition).”
  
      The authors illustrate this unprecedented
  emotional synergy effect via analysis of “awe,”
  “nostalgia,” and “humor” or the “joke.” 
  “Awe” results from the clashing together (echoes of
  perspective by incongruity?) of “fear” and an
  “appreciation of beauty or grandeur.”  Our weakness
  and vulnerability put in the context of a greatness of
 power
  or achievement far beyond our own results in a novel
  feeling, one never before experienced on planet earth. 
  “Awe” is an especially central ingredient in religious
  faith and ritual, as well as elation, and a glowing sense
 of
  transcendence and spiritual renewal.
  
      “Nostalgia” emerges from a
  juxtaposition of “past happiness” and sadness over
  “present or potential loss,” combined with an
  “imagined,” possibly future, denouement.  I’ve
  already referred to this effect, since cognition
 accompanies
  this emotional synergy.  As Freud and Norman O. Brown
  argue, a sense of “time” is constructed in just such a
  dramatically progressive manner.
  
      The most readily understandable example
  of cognitive-emotional synergy Deacon and Cashman advance
 is
  that for “humor.”  The “emotional eruption”
  here produced, the authors demonstrate, by “incompatible
  arousal states,” can be easily appreciated by any
 Burkean,
  indeed any jokester.  A sense of “irony” would be
  closely associated with this synergistic convergence.
  
      The “eureka” moment the authors
  additionally elaborate on combines partly similar, partly
  disparate, emotional roots.
  
      More later, perhaps, in summary and with
  related reflections.
  
  
      Ed       
  
  --------------------------------------------
  On Wed, 3/18/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
  wrote:
  
   Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
   To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
   Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
  <kb at kbjournal.org>
   Date: Wednesday, March 18, 2015, 4:45 PM
   
   Burkophiles,
   
       Once more:
   
       According to Deacon and Cashman, the
   second of the three “synergies” that accompanied
 the
   evolution of language, and that helps undergird the
   religious sensibilities of the symbolic species, is
 the
   symbol-induced tropism toward conception of “a
  bi-layered
   world,” i.e., perception of “a
   pattern-behind-the-pattern,” an almost compulsive
   “metaphysical dualism” common to religions in
 general.
   
       This two-world metaphysics---analogous to
   Burke’s assertion that language is a
 “transcendence”
   in and of itself, irrespective of other, more common
  notions
   of transcendence language may evoke---this two-world
   metaphysics goes something like this:
   
       Symbol-users, like nonsymbolic animals,
   live in world of “real,” “material,”
 “tactile
  and
   visible objects and living beings,” a world of
  “concrete
   objects and events.”  Unlike those reptilian and
   mammalian precursors, however, symbolizers inhabit a
   “second world” as well.  This underlying world
 is
   one of “symbols that are linked together by
 meaningful
   associations,” a “virtual,” indeed a
  “spiritual,”
   world.  The symbolic species is, consequently, a
   “bi-layered” being.  Just as the religious
   sensibility completes, in a sense, the
 symbol-generated
   “narrative” union of mammalian procedural and
 episodic
   memory systems (religion fulfilling most
 “perfectly”
  the
   thrust toward a narrative “telos,” or redemptive
   consummation, of a morally-tinged, guilt-inaugurated
   troublesome tale, to “Burke” the matter a bit),
 so
   religious speculation (call it “imagination” if
  that’s
   your estimate) brings to closure a
    most satisfying harmony between these two disparate
   “realities”: life “bottomed on the earth,”
 to
  quote
   Melville, and the transcendental world of symbolic
   relationships and implications.
   
       At its foundation, this “second,”
   “spiritually” symbolic world, is epitomized by,
 or
   illustrated via, humankind’s “dictionary” or
   “thesaurus” syndrome.  It’s the
 tunnel-vision,
   mere “iconic” and “indexical” communication
 of the
   other mammals that functions on the basis of
 one-to-one,
   signifier-signified relationships, diametrically
 different
   from the dictionary/thesaurus obsession of our
   species.  The “lower” animals “see” a
   significant form (the icon) and “point” to it by
 way
  of
   a physical or vocal gesture (the index).  This kind
 of
   index, the generically mammalian index, “maps
 onto”
  the
   object or event one-to-one.  It’s a “sinsign,”
 to
   borrow from Charles Sanders Peirce, which Deacon
 does. 
   The indexical sinsign is “particular,” not
   “general.”  There’s no need for learned
   skillfulness in “disambiguating” reference. 
   “Subject/predicate,” “topic/comment,”
 “indexical
    operation/symbolic operation” quandaries do not
   apply.  I dealt with these matters before in posts
   9/16/14 and 10/6/14.
   
       In the communication of the symbolic
   species, there are no “sinsigns,” only
  “legisigns,”
   or generalized signs, to further elaborate on
 Peirce. 
   The relationship at the heart of the symbolizers’
   communication is signifier-signifier, not
   signifier-signified.  Their SYMBOLIC communication
 is
   based on a “system-internal web of
 relationships”
   requiring “an associated indexical operation . . .
 in
   order to point outside this system.”
   
       This in-the-mind system of symbolic
   relationships is the “other” world, the
 “second”
   world, homo sapiens/homo loqax dwells in.  As I
 said
   before, symbols are not, Deacon claims, mere
 “code,”
   “sign,” “icon,” or number, that is, symbols
 are
  not
   just pointers, markers, gauges, or portraits  of
 the
   kind so often denominated “symbols” in common
   parlance.  Actual symbols refer, abstractly and
   generally, “irrespective of any natural
   affinities.”  (See GM, where Burke acknowledges
 that
   each of his pentadic terms refers to “no
 thing.”) 
   In other words, as per Burke, symbols synthesize,
   synthetically, disparate beings, entities, or events
 for
   seemingly pragmatic, culturally-conditioned purposes
 that
   transcend mere appearances of similarity.  Contra
   Saussure, symbolic reference cannot be
 “mapped.” 
   To the extent that a common word or symbol
 “maps”
   anything, it maps a POSITION IN A GIVEN LEXICON IN
  RELATION
   TO OTHER
    TERMINOLOGIES IN THAT SYMBOL SYSTEM.
    
       Maybe a fair illustration of this
   symbolic proclivity is how readily our
 “episodic”
  memory
   “downloads” various synonyms to insert into the
 slots
   our “procedural” memory vouchsafes as we “ride
 the
   bike” of sentence formation, even in impromptu
 speech.
   
       Deacon and Cashman assure us that
   Children pick up on the vagaries of these “second
  world”
   relationships with ready facility, just the way they
 do so
   in regard to symbolic indexicality, reference,
 transition,
   etc.  (Deacon is now working on a coauthored book
 with
   a linguist.  An apparently anti-Chomskyan tome, it
 will
   show that reference to neither nature nor nurture is
  needed
   to demonstrate how the “rules” of grammar are
 fixed
   early on in the communicative practice of the
 symbolic
   species.)
   
       Once again, the religious imagination
   brings to climactic fruition the promptings of the
   “second” or “spiritual” world of symbols, as
 it
   confronts, bounces off of, the exigencies and
 challenges
   posed by the “material” world of potential and
   inevitable mental and physical hard
   knocks.   As Carrol says, the things that are
   “related” via symbolism may be “real,” but
 the
   relations themselves are not real.  Maybe so.  To
   apply here what Burke says in the Rhetoric, however,
 call
   this propensity, even “compulsion,” to follow the
 cues
   of language through even to the “end of the
 line,”
  call
   this “affliction” the ultimate error of the
 dialectic,
   if you will.  That need not concern us.  This is
   how symbolizers think.  Emersed as they are in this
   “hidden” realm of symbolic inducements and
  associations,
   they follow this yellow brick road (“the hidden
 [even
   “idiosyncratic”] logic of relationships behind
   symbols,” Deacon and
    Cashman call it) in myriad, labyrinthine directions,
   according to its most alluring incentives. 
   “Burking” this virtual thoroughfare, we travel by
 way
  of
   the pentad/hexad, which inevitably bleeds into the
   disorder/guilt/redemption cycle (see RM, p. 276;
 LASA, pp.
   54-55, and the first three chapters of the Primer)
   .
       Deacon and Cashman find “expression of
   [the] deeper hidden realities” beneath the
 “surface”
   appearances of the physical world in the
 “Dreamline”
   myths of Australian aborigines, readings of entrails
 by
   Pagan priests, the Hindu double-world tradition of
 the
   “maya,” Plato’s ideal forms, Acquinas’s
 “Pure
   Ideas” in the Mind of God, and the connective
  trajectories
   to the Mind of God in the “Western science” of
  Galileo,
   Kepler, and Einstein.
   
       I ask: Wouldn’t ALL philosophical
   speculation be part and parcel with this
 “virtual,”
   “spiritual,” empirically un-“real,” perhaps
 even
   “idiotically” savant-skewered realm of potential
   illusion?
   
       The thired “synergy” next time, the
   symbolic blending of primary mammalian emotions into
 the
   religiously complex.
   
   
       Ed       
                  
           
   
   --------------------------------------------
   On Mon, 3/16/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
   wrote:
   
    Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
    To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
    Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
   <kb at kbjournal.org>
    Date: Monday, March 16, 2015, 2:44 PM
    
    Burkophiles,
    
        According to Deacon and Cashman, the
    second of the three “synergies” that accompanied
  the
    evolution of language, and that undergird the
  religious
    sensibilities of the symbolic species, is the
   symbol-induced
    tropism toward conception of “a bi-layered
  world,”
   i.e.,
    perception of “a pattern-behind-the-pattern,” an
   almost
    compulsive “metaphysical dualism” common to
  religions
   in
    general.”
    
        At the outset, let me say I tend to read
    with the cluster/agon procedure at least in the back
  of my
    mind.  To some extent, I’m checking the
    what-goes-with-what, the what-vs.-what, and the
  from-what
    through-what to-what.  I did no formal cluster work
  on
    “The Origins of Religion,” but I’ve tried to
  put 2
   and
    2 together with Burke’s method in mind 
    
        So, some of the things I’ll say here
    are “inferences,” as in “Fact, Inference, and
  Proof
   in
    the Analysis of Literary Symbolism” (1954). 
  Deacon
    and Cashman nowhere say that religion is a pox on
  the
    symbol-using animal.  But I think in this, the
  second
    section of the body of their treatise, the authors
  perhaps
    hint at a dismissive attitude toward this
   inevitable---their
    take---human singulaity.
    
        Recall what I said in a previous post:
    The most common scientific explanation for religion
    references its perhaps once-useful, but now
  nonadaptive,
    qualities as a genre of symbolic action.  Religion
  is a
    “by-product,” the Goulds, Lewontins, and
  Dawkinses
   say,
    a “misapplication” of a kind.  Deacon and
    Cashman’s critique is that this notion is way too
    superficial.  However, D and C allow that their
  richer
    and more nuanced treatment can incorporate something
  of
   that
    orthodox view.  In this division of their article,
  the
    authors might, in fact, be so incorporating.
    
        As I infer, reading a bit between the
    lines, over the course of a long evolutionary
  history,
    hominids lost a great deal of the sensory acuity of
   nonhuman
    and prehuman mammals, but, compensatorily, acquired,
  via
    natural selection, a linguistic facility that
  overmatched
    that deficit.  Symbols enabled an exponentially
  more
    supple, varied, and potent means of manipulating
  natural
    resources in service to human ends than anything that
  went
    before.  Along with that new “tool,”
  however,”
    came the “by-product,” a terministic link, Deacon
  and
    Cashman make, back to the orthodox schema, a reader
  would
    presume.
    
        Another hint of a dislogistic sort in
    this second section on the symbolic “bi-layered
  world”
    is how Deacon, along with Cashman, handle the term
    “symbolic savant.”  I made reference to this
  term
    in a previous post.  Deacon employes it in another
  of
    his articles on the “Symbolic Species.”  There,
  in
    contrast, the expression stands alone in its
  grandeur. 
    Here, “symbolic savant” is explicitly joined at
  the
   hip
    with the analogous term it so readily brings to
  mind:
    “idiot savant.”  “Idiot savants” can
  wondrously
    perform cognitive feats that flabbergast the rest of
  us,
    like solve complex math problems in an instant.  But
  in
    most other arenas of life, they’re not so
  proficient.
    
        Add to these dribs and drabs of cold
    water the authors may be splashing on religion two
  more
    points: Deacon and Cashman conclude here that the
   religious
    sensibility they describe is altogether “natural”
  in
    origin.  Nothing “supernatural” is involved. 
    That’s surely what we would expect.  Deacon adds
  in
    his second Ginn Lecture, though, that
  transcendentalizing
    “teleology” is “redundant.”  Exactly what
    Deacon means by that isn’t clear.  But its
    implication is.
    
        This intro to the second division of
    “Role of Symbolic Capacity in the Origins of
  Religion”
    took longer than I expected.  I’ll get to the
  details
    of the section part of the authors’ argument in the
  next
    post.
    
    
        Ed
    
    
        P.S. I forgot to mention, in this section
    of “Origins of Religion,” Deacon and Cashman add
    “music” as part and parcel of this
  “by-product”
    scenario.  “Music,” the authors say, is a
    “semiotic vehicle able to transport us into a world
  of
    fluid half emotions lying behind and evoked by the
    sounds.”  Interestingly, mega-orthodox
  evolutionists
    Stephen Jay Gould and Richard Lewontin add female
  orgasms
   to
    religion and music as “nonadaptive byproducts” of
  the
    evolutionary process (“The Spandrels of San Marco
  and
   the
    Panglossian Paradigm: A Critique of the Adaptionist
    Programme,” 1979).  I’ve always said on this
    matter, check first with a paid soprano in a church
    choir.             
         
    
    --------------------------------------------
    On Fri, 3/13/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
    wrote:
    
     Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
     To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
     Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
    <kb at kbjournal.org>
     Date: Friday, March 13, 2015, 4:36 PM
     
     Burkophiles,
     
         On the “synergy” that generates
     narrative, the first of the three ways evolved
   symbolic
     capacity transformed the pre-human mammalian
 brain
   into
    the
     powerful force for “action,” and theotropic
    speculation
     and motivation, it is today, according to Deacon
 and
     Cashman:
     
         All mammals (maybe birds, as well),
     Deacon and Cashman say, possess two mnemonic
 systems,
   two
     means of generating the memory that makes for
   enhanced
     adaptation to an environment and shifting
     circumstances.  One mnemonic agency is
     “procedural.”  The other is “episodic.” 
 D
     and C label the procedural, seemingly the
   lower-level,
     apparatus for retaining memory traces,
     “diachronic.”  The diachronic is concerned
 with,
   or
     pertains to, the historical DEVELOPMENT of
   something. 
     It is not of a uniform age or time.  Hence,
   diachronic
     or procedural memory fixes in the mammalian brain
   the
    means
     for rote motor skills, habits, eventually
 automatic
     responses that become “second nature,” as
   repeated
    again
     and again,  Procedural memory particularly
 enlists
   the
     basal ganglia, the motor cortex, and the
   cerebellum. 
     (These would appear to be lower-order
     neuro-mechanisms.  Yet, Deacon has said to me in
     correspondence that he does not hold to any
 strict
   notion
      of the tripartite brain.  This stance would seem
  to
     suggest that the brain works as something of a
 whole,
   an
     integrated unit of a sort.  So, I would need
   further
     clarification from Deacon on distinctions between
     “higher” and “lower” neuro-operations, a
    distinction
     he and Cashman seem to be making in this article.
     )
         “Episodic” memory, which I will call
     here a higher-order mnemonic system (after D and
 C,
   note
     below) is “synchronic,” Deacon and Cashman
   assert. 
     Episodic memory pertains to “individual events
 and
     relationships.”  It has to do with the state
 of
     something at one particular time.  Though
 “classes
   of
     episodic memories” can be retained, the
   “different
     [neuro] structures” involved make for a
   “different
    kind
     of redundancy” in respect to the episodic. 
   “Serial
     redundancy is unavailable.”  The hippocampus
 is
     integral to the “distributive” nature of
   episodic
     retention.  “The hippocampus is reciprocally
     connected with many different areas of
 generalized
     neocortex.”  Nested in the cerebrum, it
   “correlates
     converging information from higher-order
 processing
   in
    each
     sensory modality.”  Episodic “redundancy”
 is
     found “in context to other events to which it
 was
     linked.”
     
         Now, as already indicated, all mammals,
     maybe birds, also, benefit from both kinds
 memory,
   the
     procedural and the episodic.  In those
 nonsymbolic
     animals, however, the two memory systems are
   separated,
    not
     integrated.  The symbolic capacity that the
   emerging
     species homo sapiens evolved into across the
    2-million-year
     Quaternary brought the two mnemonic systems into
 a
     revolutionary synergy.  It is symbols that
   integrated
     and integrate the two types of memory.  The
   following
     is the result:  This is how it is done:
     
     “        Syntax,” word order,
     “becomes part of our procedural memory
   system.” 
     “When we produce a sentence, it is a bit like
   riding a
     bike . . . . It is proceduralized.  But what we
 are
     doing with it is accessing and ‘downloarding’
     information from episodic memory in order to
 express
   an
    idea
     or accomplish a communicative action.”
     
             Thus, “the interplay between
     the serial [that is, the procedural] and
   distributive
     associative [i.e., the episodic] tendencies
 brought
   into
     interaction by language provides a way to
 organize
    episodic
     memory into sequences.”  And so, “the
 narrative
     predisposition [that results] can be understood
 as
   an
     emergent consequence of the unique mnemonic
 synergy
   that
     language has made possible.”
     So aver Deacon and Cashman.
     
             Now, what is distinctive about
     the ensuing “narrative” that nudges
 symbolizers
   toward
     religion, or metaphysical speculations? 
 Narrative
   is
     not merely the equivalent of the proverbial
 put-down
   of
     “history” as simply “one darned thing after
     another.”  Narrative is not just stories about
     synchronic episodes that progress according to
   diachronic
     habitual sequences.  Narratives tend to be
   peculiarly
     “directed” sequences, explanations,
   interpretations,
     allegations, justifications, commentaries,
 personal
   or
    group
     representations of the facts of the matter, made
 via
    causal
     connections, with indictment or praise for the
   parties
     involved (Deacon & Cashman; Shorter OED). 
   Indeed,
     narratives can generate “a sense of present or
   potential
     loss” that fuses “present, past, and imagined
     experiences” (Deacon & Cashman) in a way
 that
    conjures
     Neo-Freudian Norman O. Brown’s dramatistic
   explanation
    of
     how human narration creates “time.”
       In the “id” there is no time, says
 Freud. 
     In the ego, however, “Time has to be constructed
 by
   an
     animal that has guilt [or a sense of loss] and
 seeks
   to
     expiate [or redeem or correct that sense of
   loss].” 
     Such a being must create and dwell on the notions
 of
   a
     “past” and a “future.”  “Archaic man
   [sic;
     and modern man and woman] experiences guilt and
   therefore
     time” (Brown, Life Against Death: The
   Psychoanalytical
     Meaning of History, 1959, pp. 274, 276, 278,
   87-109).`
     
             Even more to the point than the
     inherently dramatic nature of narrative plot
 lines
   and
     narrative’s temporal fusion of “loss” with
 a
   vision
    of
     something redemptively better, is the stark
 contrast
    between
     the “telos” essential to the narrative
 impulse,
   and
    the
     lack of such consummation in many, can we not say
   all,
    human
     lives.  The sense of narrative near impels
   symbolizers,
     Deacon and Cashman contend, toward transcendental
   plot
     lines.  “They [narratives] do not simply stop
     arbitrarily, as do most lives,” Deacon and
 Cashman
     observe.  “Instead, in the narrative of a
 life,
     whether real or imagined, birth and death are
 events
   that
     are usually subordinated to some telos, . . .
 .” 
   The
     gross narrative of a human life as actually
 lived,
    abridged
     of any larger meaning in itself, tends to be
    “embedded”
     in, or related to, a larger narrative that
   overmatches the
     glaring limitations of the “brief candle” of
   human
     existence.
     
             “Telos”: “End, purpose,
     (an) ultimate object or aim” (Shorter OED).
     
             Later, two more “synergies,”
     generated by language, that might well help to
   explain
    still
     further the otherwise strange otherworldliness of
   the
     symbol-using animal.
     
     
             Ed       
                    
            
            
     
     --------------------------------------------
     On Thu, 3/12/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
     wrote:
     
      Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
      To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
      Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
     <kb at kbjournal.org>
      Date: Thursday, March 12, 2015, 3:53 PM
      
      Burkophiles,
      
          At the outset of their article, “The
      Role of Symbolic Capacity in the Origins of
    Religion”
      (2009), Terrence W. Deacon and his coauthor
  Tyrone
    Cashman
      cite polling data that 86 percent of human beings
    believe
     in
      God or some sort of Supreme Being.  Burke, as
    I’ve
      argued, gives good reasons why that is the case
      IMPLICITLY.  From a very different academic
  vantage
      point, Deacon and Cashman do so EXPLICITLY. 
    Language
      is central in the case they, the three of them,
  make
    for
     the
      ubiquity of belief in the god-term, and the case
  for
    the
      global occurrence of belief in the Divine, as
     characterized
      in many variant ways.
      
          Deacon, the biological
      anthropologist/neuroscientist (I know nothing
  about
     Cashman)
      is coming at the relationship between symbolic
    capacity
     and
      the religious “predisposition” very much from
  an
      evolutionary and neuroscientific angle.  One
    notable
      point needs made at the outset: Forget about homo
      sapiens/homo dialecticus, language in tow,
  suddenly
      appearing on the scene circa 200,000 years ago. 
  D
    and
      C say language development and brain size-and-
    structure
      development gradually and reciprocally occurred
  over
    the
      entire Pleistocene, which is to say, across the
    whole
      near-2-million-year duration of the Quaternary
    Period. 
      Brain evolution gradually brought linguistic
  facility
    into
      being, while, at the same time, emerging symbolic
    capacity
      reciprocally altered the human brain.
      
          (Sounds a bit analogous to the
      Wallace/Lovelock/Gaia hypothesis on reciprocal
    changes in
      both life and the nonliving material scene over
  3.5
     billion
      years, but that’s another matter.)
      
          At the outset, also, the coauthors
      summarize the three most-cited theories about the
    origin
     of
      religion via evolutionary change: religion as
      “nonadaptive,” or useless, by-product of
    formerly
      adaptive changes, “misapplications,” so to
  speak,
    of
      evolved tendencies; more or less “adaptive”
    proofs
      against mortality; and “parasitic memes”
    concocted
      socially, rather than individually, as oppressive
     sanctions
      of one kind or another.  The first of the above
  is
    the
      most favored.  All these explanations are
    inadequate
      “reductionistic” takes on this universal
  human
      phenomenon, Deacon and Cashman contend.  
      To get to the pith and marrow of the issue, as
  Deacon
    and
      Cashman see it: With the evolution of language,
    three
      “synergies” emerged that made religious
    speculation
     and
      belief, as well as the general “metaphysical”
    search
     for
      underlying “First Principles,” an inevitable
     consequence
      of biological change  (“Metaphysics” for
      Burke?  “Coy theology,” you will recall).
      
          “Synergy”: “The production by two
      or more agents, substances [structures,
  capacities,
    etc.]
     of
      a combined effect greater than the sum or their
    separate
      effects . . . Increased effectiveness or
  achievement
      produced by combined action.”
      
          “Emergent,” quite similar in meaning:
      “An effect produced by a combination of causes
  but
     unable
      to be seen as the sum of their individual
  effects”
      (Shorter OED).
      
          Three “synergies,” occasioned by the
      evolution of symbol use over that long span of
  time,
     brought
      into being this “religious” or
    “metaphysical”
     being,
      the “symbolic species,” making religion, D and
  C
     assert,
      humankind’s “synergy of synergies.”  These
      combinations of, or symbol-generated interactions
    between,
      structures and capacities our mammalian ancestors
     possessed
      and utilized, are: 
      (1)    “The role of language in a novel
      synergy between 2 previously orthogonal modes of
    memory
      storage which is the basis for the narrative
     predisposition
      that is distinctively characteristic of human
    reasoning,
      identity, and culture.”
      
      (2)    A tropism toward conception of what D
      and C call a “bi-layered world,” namely, an
    “evolved
      attentional bias toward discerning a pattern
      behind-the-pattern, a bias required for language
    learning,
      which makes metaphysical dualism intuitively
  natural,
    and
      also makes the double-world metaphysics common to
    most
      religions a likely leap of symbolic
  imagination.”
      
      (3)     “The dramatic expansion
      and transformation of the mammalian emotional
    repertoire
     by
      virtue of the use of symbolic blends to induce
     unprecedented
      interactions and novel experiential synergies
  that
    we
      describe as emergent emotional experiences.”
      
      “Orthogonal”: As inferred from Deacon’s work
  as
    a
      whole, the meaning here, I would assume, is
    “straight,
      normal, proper [I think we can add
  “natural”],
    without
      external influence,” “completely
  independent,”
    which
      is to say, “free” of influence by the
    “absential
      feature,” or nonsymbolic negative, that
  nonhuman
    living
      beings are beholden to.  Hence, we’re talking
    here
      about the  “orthograde.”  “Orthograde,”
  in
      Deaconese, serves, it seems to me, as the
  opposite
    of
      “contragrade.”  “Contragrade” refers to
  a
    force
      that runs counter to mechanical nonliving
  processes.
       
          Yet, there seems to me to be an ambiguity
      here with this term.  “Orthogonal,” seen as
  a
      derivative of “orthograde,” would likely
    characterize
      nonsymbolic living beings, as well as symbolic. 
    Lower
      animals and plants appear to be
  “contragrade,”
    too, in
      the sense that they uniquely resist entropy by
  way
    of
      reproduction and photosynthesis, or by
  reproduction
    and
      direct or indirect ingestion of the products of
      photosynthesis.  Maybe I can get Deacon to bring
  me
    up
      to speed on the definition “orthogonal,” as
    employed
      here, when I forward the post to him.
      
          I’ll try to unpack the three synergies
      in subsequent posts.
      
      
          Ed      
                     
          
      
      --------------------------------------------
      On Fri, 3/6/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
      wrote:
      
       Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
       To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
       Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
      <kb at kbjournal.org>
       Date: Friday, March 6, 2015, 8:57 PM
       
       Clarke,
       
       Thanks for your extended and
       insightful reply to my post.  Hey, thanks for
   even
      reading
       my long rant!  You certainly make some good
   points,
      though
       I would interpret the "potentiality" claim in
 RM
       as making our emergence out of a "wordless"
   Ground
       as maybe "more complicated" than that. 
   Following
       Burke, I could quote Scripture here, a passage
 in
     Romans
      and
       one in Hebrews on how the visible things of
 this
     world
       adumbrate a reality that is beyond.  I'll let
     Deacon
       expand on the matter in his own way in a
   subsequent
     post.
       
       (Not that Deacon is
       necessarily a theist.  The religious
 sensibility
     that
      comes
       so "naturally" to the "symbolic species"
       is, for Deacon, just that: inevitable, yes,
 but
     natural. 
       "Teleology" is real, one of the last frames in
   his
       second Ginn Lecture proclaims, but to
     transcendentalize it
       is "redundant, Deacon avers.  An
 unsatisfactory
       denouement for the "symbol-users" or the
       "symbolic species," i.e., coming at the matter
       from either Burke's perspective, or
 Deacon's. 
       That's why 86 percent of Americans believe in
   God,
     or
       some such figure, as Deacon and Cashman note
 at
   the
       beginning of their journal article.)
       
       By way of illustration, I offered in one of my
       posts to Deacon the characters in Samuel
 Becket's
     black
       comedy, "Waiting for Godot."  At the end of
 the
       play, they are still waiting, and the
   implications
     are
      that
       Godot is not going to show up, yet these
 forlorn
     wretches
       will continue to wait . . and wait . . . and
 wait
   . .
     .
      and
       never stop waiting.  
       
       On
       the symbol-user's tendency to "see" drama in,
       or superimpose drama on, the supposedly blind
   motions
     of
      the
       universe---Deacon has a powerful explanation
 for
     that,
      which
       I'll get to eventually.
       
       Thanks again for replying---and rebutting!
       
       
       
       Ed
       
       
       
       
       
       
       
        
       --------------------------------------------
       On Fri, 3/6/15, Clarke Rountree <rountrj at uah.edu>
       wrote:
       
        Subject: Re: [KB]
       Burke, Deacon, and Theology
        To:
       "Edward C Appel" <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
        Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
       <kb at kbjournal.org>
        Date: Friday, March 6, 2015, 12:23 PM
        
        Dear Burkelers--
        My good friend Ed obviously
       
       has a passion for seeing Burke as a theologian
        (coy or otherwise), despite Burke’s
       insistence that he
        isn’t a believer in
       God.
        Of course one can get around the
       problem of Burke not
        actually believing in
       God through the
        Spinozan strategy of making
       God equal to Nature, so that the
        GROUND of
       things
        itself is equivalent to God. And,
       notably, if verbalizing is
        somehow
       contained
        in or implicit in that GROUND,
       then, Ed reasons, that is an
        acknowledgment
       of
        at least a God principle at work. Even
       an old atheist like
        me will admit that
        we humans, surprisingly, arose from a wordless
       world, that
        post facto we know carried the
       potentiality for
        creating wordy
        creature. Note, also, that Burke insists that
       when humans
        are gone it will go
        back to its wordlessness. (I’m speaking of
       Earth only, of
        course; I think it
        perfectly obvious that there is sentient life
       elsewhere in
        the universe.)
        
        
        Last year
       my son John and
        I published our essay from
       the
        Burke conference in Belgium where we
       argued about the
        potentialities and
        dangers of the motivational bias of humans.
       (We called it a
        symbol-users
        guide. I don't have the citation handy,
       but I can get it
        if anyone's
       interested.) That is, building on my
       
       “Dramatism as Literal” essay in KBJ
 (2010),
   we
        noted that it is intrinsic to humans to see
       the world
        through the grammar of
        motives, and that this grammar becomes a
       problematic
        terministic screen. (It
        also is a wonderful thing, of course, as it
       makes us
        recognizably human—my point
        in the 2010 essay.) After mulling over that
       issue, I have a
        new appreciation
        for Burke’s suggestion that toddlers may
       learn the idea of
        “No” before they
        learn the idea of “nothing.” The
       implication is that
        “scientific” or
       “objective”
        understandings of the world
       are an add-on—something
        secondary we have
       to learn,
        while the search for motives is
       primary.
        
        
        Now, early humans learned
        to
       search for motives in prey and
        in animals
       that preyed on them. Scientists may tell us
 now
        that animals don’t
        have the
       capacity for the kind of actions we may
 attribute
        to them, but
        thinking that
       way may have been beneficial in making us wary
        of what animals
        are doing
       when they run or attack. Of course, walking
   around
        with a “pentadic”
        set of
       glasses on the world undoubtedly gives rise to
        animism and, later, more
       
       complex forms of religion, with gods in the
   river,
     the
       sea,
        the sky, etc.
        
         
        
        (Let me
       add quickly as
        well, that the dramatistic
       screen did not handicap humans
        in
       developing technology and improving their lot;
     it’s
        just that every agency
        was
       connected to a human purpose, every scene
   implicitly
        asked “what can be
        done
       [for humans] with this?” Indeed, that's one
 of
        the warning John and I make--that we have
       trouble seeing
        anything except as it
       relates to us.)
        
        
        When scientists look at
        the
       world as objects, they blind
        themselves to
       this primary way of thinking (though, as Burke
        says, not so much that
        they
       don’t know to treat their fellow chemists
     differently
        than the chemicals
        with which
       they work). When people want to look at more
 than
        things,
        investigating the
       human and the social, wearing these
       
       blinders threaten to make
        them miss the
       FUNCTIONS of pentadic screens in human
       
       interactions.
        
        
        Now Burke, looking at
       
       religion, would be missing much if he
        were
       so blinded; but of course he is not. He not
 only
        accounts for the key term
       
       ACTION in human relations (while excoriating
   those
     who
       focus
        on MOTION), but he
       
       investigates the potentialities of symbolic
   action.
     That
        this leads him to find
        a
       particularly potent (and “perfected”)
   symbolic
     form
       in
        religion is
       
       unsurprising—religion is one of the oldest,
   most
        scrutinized, most pervasive,
       
       most defended, most argued over, and (thus)
 most
       perfected
        symbolic systems we
        have. 
        
        
        Ultimately, I believe that
       
       the perfectedness of religion as
        a symbol
       system has nothing to do with its intrinsic
 truth
        (except as, perhaps, an "end of the
       line" human
        truth, an implication of
       our terminology). It does have much
        to do
       with its relation to power, making it a key
 bone
   of
        contention for those
        who
       would rise to prominence in social systems.
 And
        Burke’s focus on religion
       
       has more to do with the fact that, given its
   intense
        scrutiny by some of the
       
       brightest minds for millennia (at least since
     Augustine)
       it
        is the most thoroughgoing
        symbolic system around. However, as I noted in
       a QJS
        article on the construction of George
       H.W. Bush in the
        1992
       
       presidential elections (1995), there are other
   well
        developed symbol systems that have
        pushed the envelope of what is “thinkable”
       through our
        dramatistic grammar—my key
        example, criminal law, where hundreds of years
       of
        Anglo-American law (and
       
       earlier law as well) helped to refine the
     possibilities
       for
        guilt and
        innocence.
       Had Burke spent more time talking to law
 folks,
        maybe he would have
        landed on
       criminal law as a perfection of symbol systems
        (though, note, he does
        spend
       a lot of time with constitutions!). That
   wouldn’t
        make him a lawyer; and
       
       finding perfection in religion doesn’t make
 him
   a
        theologian. Just an admirer
       
       of what has been wrought.
        
       
       
        That’s my oar in the
       
       water.
        
        
       
       (On a side note, I’m a
        great fan of
       Ed’s explication of
        Deacon’s work and
       its implications for understanding
       
       Burke’s work. I look forward to the next
       
       installment.)
        Cheers,
       
       Clarke
        On Thu, Mar 5, 2015 at 2:52
        PM, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
        wrote:
        Burkophiles,
        
        
        
                I’ve posted at length here on
       intersections
        between Burke and Terrence W.
       Deacon, Chair of the
        Department of
       Anthropology at Cal Berkeley, you may
       
       recall.  (Of equal importance, Deacon is a
       neuroscientist,
        as well.)  In response to
       a recent e-mail of mine on his
        aptly
       described “tour de force,” Incomplete
 Nature:
     How
        Mind Emerged from Matter (Norton, 2012, 2013),
       Professor
        Deacon kindly sent me additional
       materials of his: two more
        of his published
       articles, bringing my cache to seven in
       
       toto; and several series of exceedingly well
   wrought
        powerpoint frames and other visuals he has
       used in lectures
        at the University of Oslo
       in Norway, in Holland, the Ginn
        Lectures in
       Atlanta, and a couple of presentations to
       
       theologians.
        
        
        
                As some of you
       will likely suspect, the theology
        theme
       immediately piqued my interest.  Here we
 have,
   it
        seemed to me, another important point of
       convergence between
        Burke and Deacon,
       adding to the considerable list I’ve
       
       already outlined.   The focal reference here
   will
     be to
        Deacon’s coauthored article (with Tyrone
       Cashman), “The
        Role of Symbolic Capacity
       in the Origins of Religion,”
        Journal of
       Religion, Nature & Culture, 2009, Vol. 3
 No.
        4, pp. 490-517.
        
        
        
             
         I want to begin, in this post, with my take
 on
        Burke’s anfractuous relationship to
       theology, then in a
        subsequent edition,
       summarize Deacon’s position.
        
        
        
             
         Burke, we know, claimed publicly that his
        interest in theology was entirely secular. 
       “Logology,”
        the late-Burke title for
       his philosophy looked at as an
       
       “epistemology,” was solely about the
 contours
   of
        symbolic action, its motives and tendentious
       operations, not
        about any putatively
       transcendental reality.  Logology was
        the
       “systematic study of theological terms for
 the
     light
        they might throw on the forms of language,”
       theological
        terms being the most
       thoroughgoing, far-reaching, ultimate
        terms
       in language,” language, in Burke’s pat
   phrase,
        taken to “the end of the line.”  Tim
       Crusius doubled
        down on Burke’s
       affirmation in Kenneth Burke and the
       
       Conversation After Philosophy (Southern
 Illinois
   UP,
       1999),
        and in his review of Greig
       Henderson’s book,  Kenneth
        Burke:
       Literature and Language as Symbolic Action, in
   QJS
     76
        (1990), pp. 340-342.
        
        
        
             
         Add to Burke’s official position on
 logology
        (was it, or was it not, something of a façade
       that a
        cluster/agon analysis can maneuver
       around?---I ask, and have
        asked) Burke’s
       private claim to have been a nontheist. 
       
       Wayne Booth  (“Wax ‘N Wayne,” as Burke
   would
     call
       him
        privately) was a frequent correspondent
       of KB’s.  In his
        chapter, “Kenneth
       Burke’s Religious Rhetoric:
       
       ‘God-Terms’ and the Ontological Proof,”
 in
       Rhetorical
        Invention and Religious Inquiry
       (Yale UP, 2000, pp. 25-46),
        Booth takes
       note of Burke’s demurrer.  (See, also,
 “The
        Many Voices of Kenneth Burke, Theologian and
       Prophet, as
        Revealed in His Letters to
       Me,” in Unending Conversations:
        New
       Writings by and about Kenneth Burke, Eds.
   Henderson
     and
        Williams [Southern Illinois UP, 2001, pp.
       179-201], where
        Booth offers a second
       reprise on his 1996 plenary address at
        the
       Duquesne Conference, i.e., the Third
 Triennial.)
        
        
        
                On his actual belief or nonbelief,
       I think we
        have to take Burke at his
       word.  Let’s assume he was the
        nontheist
       he told people he was.
        
       
       
        
                That having
       been said, Burke’s demonstrable
       
       theological obsession cannot be gainsaid.  In
     Literature
        and Language (1988), Henderson looked behind
       the curtain:
        “For Burke logology is in
       some sense a surrogate
        theology.”  Greig
       added, “The analogies he makes for
       
       heuristic purposes betray a psychological need
 for
   a
       sense
        of permanence akin to a religious
       faith in the curative
        power of the word
       made flesh” (p. 105).  That same year,
       
       at ECA (April 29), Trevor Melia speculated
 about
     “what
        kind of Christian” Burke was, suggested that
       the label
        “secular Christian” can only
       be a starting point, and
        concluded that
       Burke was at least “up to his ears in
       
       Christianity.”
        
        
        
                At length, I had
       already corresponded with Burke
        on the
       matter (late 1983, early 1984), my case,
   “Kenneth
        Burke: Coy Theologian,” later published in
       the Journal of
        Communication and Religion
       (September, 1993).  Burke had
        even allowed
       at the Philadelphia Conference (March,1984)
       
       that I had made a “powerful argument” that
 he
   was
     a
        theologian.  I got that news from Herb
       Simons, who was
        present at the after-hours
       discussion several key scholars
        had had
       with Burke after the first day’s seminars.
        
        
        
                Other names that can be added to
       these
        speculations about Burke and religion
       include, as per Booth
        in that Yale
       publication, Burks, Carter, Duerden, Durham,
        Freccero, Gunn, Gusfield, and Jay.  And I can
       add Richard
        Thames and Steven Mailloux to
       their number (Steve’s paper
        at the Ghent
       Conference: “Under the Sign of Theology:
       
       Kenneth Burke on Language and the Supernatural
        Order.")
        
        
        
             
         I won’t lay out my entire case on Burke
 as
        “coy theologian.”  But I’ll highlight a
       few central
        points:
        
        
        
             
         The bottom line in my correspondence with
 KB,
        and in my journal piece, was this: It
       doesn’t matter what
        Burke personally
       believed or did not believe.  When a
       
       theorist posits that symbolizers are
 inherently
        “theotropic”---I’m borrowing a term from
       Steve
        here---that theorist is at least a
       “generic”
        theologian.  A “lure” in
       rhetoric, whether in
        “error” or not,
       strongly nudges homo loquax “Upward”
       
       not only toward a “god term” in general,
 but
     also
       toward
        the most “perfectly” satisfying
       God-term of all (GM, pp.
        306; RM, pp.
       275-76, 290-91; RR).
        
        
        
                And especially
       when it can be related to extant
        or
       historic theological systems, that generic
   theology
     takes
        on some kind of shape, affords a bit of
       implicit commentary
        this ubiquitous
       attribute.  I’ve characterized Burke’s
       
       dramatism/logology as a quasi-gnostic (a
 radical
     sense of
       a
        “Fall” into language, not into a
       lustful body)
        universalism (the quest for a
       “god-term” that unites all
        of humanity,
       and a focal program aimed at “purifying”
        conflict and “war”), friendly to
       Whitehead’s process
        theology (with its
       dialectics, de-perfecting of the Godhead,
       
       and rejection of a life after death; note what
   Burke
     said
       in
        the movie shown at Airlie House,
       1993).
        
        
       
       
                Add to these pillars of
       support what I would
        conceive as Burke’s
       occasional drift into theological
       
       principles in the paradigm sense, i.e.,
   statements
     that
       have
        to do with, or surely adumbrate, the
       actual existence of a
        Divine Essence of a
       kind.  When Burke says the
        “extrahuman
       ground” out of which humans proceeded
       
       “contains the principle of personality,
 quite
   as
     it
        contains the principle of verbalizing,” that
       this
        “’nonverbal’ ground must have
       contained the
        ‘potentiality’ of the
       verbal, otherwise the verbal could
        not have
       emerged from it,” Burke has crossed the line,
 I
        would suggest, into theology or religion
       proper (RM, pp.
        289-90).
       
       
        
        
           
           An analogous proposition is found in the
        Calabi-Yau version of string theory via the
       “anthropic
        principle,” the “notion
       that the observed laws of nature
        must be
       consistent with the presence of intelligent
 life
        and, specifically, the presence of intelligent
       observers
        like us.  Put in other terms,
       the universe looks the way it
        does because
       if conditions were even slightly different,
       
       life would not have formed and humans would
 not
   be
     around
       to
        observe it” (Shing-Tung Yau and Steve
       Nadis, The Shape of
        Inner Space: String
       Theory and the Geometry of the
        Universe’s
       Hidden Dimensions , Basic Books, 2010, p.
       
       345).
        
        
       
       
                Analogous, also, as I see it,
       is this quotation
        highlighted in one of
       Deacon’s two Ginn Lectures: “We
        need an
       understanding of nature such that it is not
   absurd
        to say that it has us as its products,” by
       Belgian chemist
        Ilya Prigogine and the
       physicist Victor J. Stenger. 
       
       (“Naturalizing Teleology: The Redemption of
   Science
     by
       the
        Rediscovery of Self and Value,”
       Deacon, 2014.)
        
        
        
                Illustrative of
       what different observers will
        “see” in
       a given statement, question, lacuna, or
       
       phenomenon, Stenger was one of the “new
     atheists,”
        author of God: The Failed Hypothesis: How
       Science Shows That
        God Does not Exist,
       2007.  Stenger, former blogger for the
       
       Huffington Post, famously said, “Science
 flies
   you
     to
       the
        moon.  Religion flies you into
       buildings.”  One could
        just as
       “truthfully” say, “Religion prepares
 meals,
        low cost or free, for the elderly and needy in
       Lancaster
        Country, Pennsylvania, and in
       other communities across the
        world.  The
       likes of godless communism and Nazism kills a
        hundred million persons, give or take a few
       tens of
        millions, in quest of a
       ‘heaven’ on earth.”
        
       
       
        
                As Burke
       says, symbols unite and divide, select
        in
       and select out, induce attention toward and
   induce
        attention away from.  Pick the blinkered
       lenses of your
        choice.  They’re all
       free!
        
        
       
       
                So, later, an examination of
       the case for the
        ubiquity of religion
       Deacon and Cashman make in the article
        that
       concludes: “We speculate that something like
 a
        religious predisposition, in the most general
       sense of the
        term, should be considered a
       universal consequence of the
        symbolic
       capacity evolving, whether here on earth, or
 in
   any
        other context where symbolic cognition might
       arise.”
        
        
        
        
        
                Ed
        
        
        
        
        
       
      
 _______________________________________________
        
        KB mailing list
        
        KB at kbjournal.org
        
        http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
        
        
        
        
        -- 
        Dr.
        Clarke Rountree
        Chair and
       Professor of
        Communication Arts
        342 Morton Hall
        University of
       Alabama in Huntsville
        Huntsville, AL 
       35899
        256-824-6646
        clarke.rountree at uah.edu
        
       
      
 _______________________________________________
       KB mailing list
       KB at kbjournal.org
       http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
       
      
      _______________________________________________
      KB mailing list
      KB at kbjournal.org
      http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
      
     
     _______________________________________________
     KB mailing list
     KB at kbjournal.org
     http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
     
    
    _______________________________________________
    KB mailing list
    KB at kbjournal.org
    http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
    
   
   _______________________________________________
   KB mailing list
   KB at kbjournal.org
   http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
   
  
  _______________________________________________
  KB mailing list
  KB at kbjournal.org
  http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
  
 
 _______________________________________________
 KB mailing list
 KB at kbjournal.org
 http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
-------------- next part --------------
A non-text attachment was scrubbed...
Name: Ev Con & D.docx
Type: application/vnd.openxmlformats-officedocument.wordprocessingml.document
Size: 27398 bytes
Desc: not available
URL: <http://kbjournal.org/pipermail/kb_kbjournal.org/attachments/20150331/edaeb342/attachment.docx>


More information about the KB mailing list