[KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology

Edward C Appel edwardcappel at frontier.com
Fri Mar 20 12:45:11 EDT 2015


Burkophiles,

	Up to this point, Deacon and Cashman say, their analysis of the emergence of religion via symbolism can fit at least in part within the “reductionistic” put-downs of most previous evolutionary schemas.  Religion can be shoehorned into the “spill-over” bin, or the “by-product” box, in the sense that “narrative” at its base does not equal religion.  Only the tendency toward a “telos” inherent in the narrative mode will lead almost inevitably in that direction, at least in terms of a capacity for conceiving of religious dimensions of life.  As for the “metaphysical dualism” theme, that scenario begins with the dictionary/thesaurus/words-in-their-inherent-and-separate-interconnectedness intuition that only secondarily, as it were, ramifies perceived underlying connections outward and upward toward some ultimately meaningful and explanatory “beyond.”  Sounds to a Burkean like the motive of perfection at work in each case, the
 dialectical “Upward Way,” the imperious negative, opening vistas of infinity, eternity, and transcendental fulfillment of one kind or another.

	The third “synergy” generated by symbols cannot be so extenuated, the authors assert.  This emergent coalescence of generic mammalian faculties that symbols induce results in complex, unprecedented emotions drawn from primary ones chimps presumably possess.   These emergent emotions include, among others, “elation,” “awe,” “equanimity,” and “spiritual renewal.”  These human transcendences result from no “simple mix.”  “Efforts to explain these phenomena as mere extrapolations from more basic adaptive mechanisms evolved for more instrumental purposes” prove unsatisfactory, the authors claim.   “Without the linkages made possible via symbols, these component [“neuronal”] processes would not interact, due to mutually exclusive generative contexts.”  Focal dynamic mechanism: “The ways symbols can reorganize cognition in unprecedented ways.
”
	Recall what I said in a previous post: Brain evolution in the direction of our species, the symbolic species, across the length of the Quaternary, was a RECIPROCAL process, the emerging symbolic faculty altering brain development, as well as brain evolution facilitating symbolization.

	As they get into the nitty-gritty of this dynamical process of complex emotional emergence, D and C make reference to more elaborated treatments of the issue, including some by Deacon himself, but still soldier on in abridged explanation.  Cognitions and accompanying emotions are the way of life for symbolizers, as well as nonsymbolizers.  Those emotional “arousal states” are considerably tamped down, though, or can be so, in human thought and  experience.  Prefrontal brain enlargement, both generating and generated by gradual evolutionary symbolic progressions, has conferred on symbolizers an “executive” capacity toward more pronounced self-control.  It, the symbolically enlarged and enhanced brain, has facilitated not only the cognitive flexibility to juxtapose and ramify myriad blends of thoughts and ideas, but also those of accompanying emotions or arousal states,  Those cognitions can be harmonized or accommodated in their disparity in a
 way that the potentially incompatible emotions cannot.  Thus, “symbolic blends can bring together otherwise incompatible or opposed component emotions.”  To put the matter another way, the “interaction” of these “blended emotional states . . . produces a synergistic effect that cannot be reduced to any simple additive relationship (i.e., not a simple 40 % angry 60 % surprised composition).”

	The authors illustrate this unprecedented emotional synergy effect via analysis of “awe,” “nostalgia,” and “humor” or the “joke.”  “Awe” results from the clashing together (echoes of perspective by incongruity?) of “fear” and an “appreciation of beauty or grandeur.”  Our weakness and vulnerability put in the context of a greatness of power or achievement far beyond our own results in a novel feeling, one never before experienced on planet earth.  “Awe” is an especially central ingredient in religious faith and ritual, as well as elation, and a glowing sense of transcendence and spiritual renewal.

	“Nostalgia” emerges from a juxtaposition of “past happiness” and sadness over “present or potential loss,” combined with an “imagined,” possibly future, denouement.  I’ve already referred to this effect, since cognition accompanies this emotional synergy.  As Freud and Norman O. Brown argue, a sense of “time” is constructed in just such a dramatically progressive manner.

	The most readily understandable example of cognitive-emotional synergy Deacon and Cashman advance is that for “humor.”  The “emotional eruption” here produced, the authors demonstrate, by “incompatible arousal states,” can be easily appreciated by any Burkean, indeed any jokester.  A sense of “irony” would be closely associated with this synergistic convergence.

	The “eureka” moment the authors additionally elaborate on combines partly similar, partly disparate, emotional roots.

	More later, perhaps, in summary and with related reflections.


	Ed       

--------------------------------------------
On Wed, 3/18/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com> wrote:

 Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
 To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
 Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org" <kb at kbjournal.org>
 Date: Wednesday, March 18, 2015, 4:45 PM
 
 Burkophiles,
 
     Once more:
 
     According to Deacon and Cashman, the
 second of the three “synergies” that accompanied the
 evolution of language, and that helps undergird the
 religious sensibilities of the symbolic species, is the
 symbol-induced tropism toward conception of “a bi-layered
 world,” i.e., perception of “a
 pattern-behind-the-pattern,” an almost compulsive
 “metaphysical dualism” common to religions in general.
 
     This two-world metaphysics---analogous to
 Burke’s assertion that language is a “transcendence”
 in and of itself, irrespective of other, more common notions
 of transcendence language may evoke---this two-world
 metaphysics goes something like this:
 
     Symbol-users, like nonsymbolic animals,
 live in world of “real,” “material,” “tactile and
 visible objects and living beings,” a world of “concrete
 objects and events.”  Unlike those reptilian and
 mammalian precursors, however, symbolizers inhabit a
 “second world” as well.  This underlying world is
 one of “symbols that are linked together by meaningful
 associations,” a “virtual,” indeed a “spiritual,”
 world.  The symbolic species is, consequently, a
 “bi-layered” being.  Just as the religious
 sensibility completes, in a sense, the symbol-generated
 “narrative” union of mammalian procedural and episodic
 memory systems (religion fulfilling most “perfectly” the
 thrust toward a narrative “telos,” or redemptive
 consummation, of a morally-tinged, guilt-inaugurated
 troublesome tale, to “Burke” the matter a bit), so
 religious speculation (call it “imagination” if that’s
 your estimate) brings to closure a
  most satisfying harmony between these two disparate
 “realities”: life “bottomed on the earth,” to quote
 Melville, and the transcendental world of symbolic
 relationships and implications.
 
     At its foundation, this “second,”
 “spiritually” symbolic world, is epitomized by, or
 illustrated via, humankind’s “dictionary” or
 “thesaurus” syndrome.  It’s the tunnel-vision,
 mere “iconic” and “indexical” communication of the
 other mammals that functions on the basis of one-to-one,
 signifier-signified relationships, diametrically different
 from the dictionary/thesaurus obsession of our
 species.  The “lower” animals “see” a
 significant form (the icon) and “point” to it by way of
 a physical or vocal gesture (the index).  This kind of
 index, the generically mammalian index, “maps onto” the
 object or event one-to-one.  It’s a “sinsign,” to
 borrow from Charles Sanders Peirce, which Deacon does. 
 The indexical sinsign is “particular,” not
 “general.”  There’s no need for learned
 skillfulness in “disambiguating” reference. 
 “Subject/predicate,” “topic/comment,” “indexical
  operation/symbolic operation” quandaries do not
 apply.  I dealt with these matters before in posts
 9/16/14 and 10/6/14.
 
     In the communication of the symbolic
 species, there are no “sinsigns,” only “legisigns,”
 or generalized signs, to further elaborate on Peirce. 
 The relationship at the heart of the symbolizers’
 communication is signifier-signifier, not
 signifier-signified.  Their SYMBOLIC communication is
 based on a “system-internal web of relationships”
 requiring “an associated indexical operation . . . in
 order to point outside this system.”
 
     This in-the-mind system of symbolic
 relationships is the “other” world, the “second”
 world, homo sapiens/homo loqax dwells in.  As I said
 before, symbols are not, Deacon claims, mere “code,”
 “sign,” “icon,” or number, that is, symbols are not
 just pointers, markers, gauges, or portraits  of the
 kind so often denominated “symbols” in common
 parlance.  Actual symbols refer, abstractly and
 generally, “irrespective of any natural
 affinities.”  (See GM, where Burke acknowledges that
 each of his pentadic terms refers to “no thing.”) 
 In other words, as per Burke, symbols synthesize,
 synthetically, disparate beings, entities, or events for
 seemingly pragmatic, culturally-conditioned purposes that
 transcend mere appearances of similarity.  Contra
 Saussure, symbolic reference cannot be “mapped.” 
 To the extent that a common word or symbol “maps”
 anything, it maps a POSITION IN A GIVEN LEXICON IN RELATION
 TO OTHER
  TERMINOLOGIES IN THAT SYMBOL SYSTEM.
  
     Maybe a fair illustration of this
 symbolic proclivity is how readily our “episodic” memory
 “downloads” various synonyms to insert into the slots
 our “procedural” memory vouchsafes as we “ride the
 bike” of sentence formation, even in impromptu speech.
 
     Deacon and Cashman assure us that
 Children pick up on the vagaries of these “second world”
 relationships with ready facility, just the way they do so
 in regard to symbolic indexicality, reference, transition,
 etc.  (Deacon is now working on a coauthored book with
 a linguist.  An apparently anti-Chomskyan tome, it will
 show that reference to neither nature nor nurture is needed
 to demonstrate how the “rules” of grammar are fixed
 early on in the communicative practice of the symbolic
 species.)
 
     Once again, the religious imagination
 brings to climactic fruition the promptings of the
 “second” or “spiritual” world of symbols, as it
 confronts, bounces off of, the exigencies and challenges
 posed by the “material” world of potential and
 inevitable mental and physical hard
 knocks.   As Carrol says, the things that are
 “related” via symbolism may be “real,” but the
 relations themselves are not real.  Maybe so.  To
 apply here what Burke says in the Rhetoric, however, call
 this propensity, even “compulsion,” to follow the cues
 of language through even to the “end of the line,” call
 this “affliction” the ultimate error of the dialectic,
 if you will.  That need not concern us.  This is
 how symbolizers think.  Emersed as they are in this
 “hidden” realm of symbolic inducements and associations,
 they follow this yellow brick road (“the hidden [even
 “idiosyncratic”] logic of relationships behind
 symbols,” Deacon and
  Cashman call it) in myriad, labyrinthine directions,
 according to its most alluring incentives. 
 “Burking” this virtual thoroughfare, we travel by way of
 the pentad/hexad, which inevitably bleeds into the
 disorder/guilt/redemption cycle (see RM, p. 276; LASA, pp.
 54-55, and the first three chapters of the Primer)
 .
     Deacon and Cashman find “expression of
 [the] deeper hidden realities” beneath the “surface”
 appearances of the physical world in the “Dreamline”
 myths of Australian aborigines, readings of entrails by
 Pagan priests, the Hindu double-world tradition of the
 “maya,” Plato’s ideal forms, Acquinas’s “Pure
 Ideas” in the Mind of God, and the connective trajectories
 to the Mind of God in the “Western science” of Galileo,
 Kepler, and Einstein.
 
     I ask: Wouldn’t ALL philosophical
 speculation be part and parcel with this “virtual,”
 “spiritual,” empirically un-“real,” perhaps even
 “idiotically” savant-skewered realm of potential
 illusion?
 
     The thired “synergy” next time, the
 symbolic blending of primary mammalian emotions into the
 religiously complex.
 
 
     Ed       
                
         
 
 --------------------------------------------
 On Mon, 3/16/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
 wrote:
 
  Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
  To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
  Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
 <kb at kbjournal.org>
  Date: Monday, March 16, 2015, 2:44 PM
  
  Burkophiles,
  
      According to Deacon and Cashman, the
  second of the three “synergies” that accompanied the
  evolution of language, and that undergird the religious
  sensibilities of the symbolic species, is the
 symbol-induced
  tropism toward conception of “a bi-layered world,”
 i.e.,
  perception of “a pattern-behind-the-pattern,” an
 almost
  compulsive “metaphysical dualism” common to religions
 in
  general.”
  
      At the outset, let me say I tend to read
  with the cluster/agon procedure at least in the back of my
  mind.  To some extent, I’m checking the
  what-goes-with-what, the what-vs.-what, and the from-what
  through-what to-what.  I did no formal cluster work on
  “The Origins of Religion,” but I’ve tried to put 2
 and
  2 together with Burke’s method in mind 
  
      So, some of the things I’ll say here
  are “inferences,” as in “Fact, Inference, and Proof
 in
  the Analysis of Literary Symbolism” (1954).  Deacon
  and Cashman nowhere say that religion is a pox on the
  symbol-using animal.  But I think in this, the second
  section of the body of their treatise, the authors perhaps
  hint at a dismissive attitude toward this
 inevitable---their
  take---human singulaity.
  
      Recall what I said in a previous post:
  The most common scientific explanation for religion
  references its perhaps once-useful, but now nonadaptive,
  qualities as a genre of symbolic action.  Religion is a
  “by-product,” the Goulds, Lewontins, and Dawkinses
 say,
  a “misapplication” of a kind.  Deacon and
  Cashman’s critique is that this notion is way too
  superficial.  However, D and C allow that their richer
  and more nuanced treatment can incorporate something of
 that
  orthodox view.  In this division of their article, the
  authors might, in fact, be so incorporating.
  
      As I infer, reading a bit between the
  lines, over the course of a long evolutionary history,
  hominids lost a great deal of the sensory acuity of
 nonhuman
  and prehuman mammals, but, compensatorily, acquired, via
  natural selection, a linguistic facility that overmatched
  that deficit.  Symbols enabled an exponentially more
  supple, varied, and potent means of manipulating natural
  resources in service to human ends than anything that went
  before.  Along with that new “tool,” however,”
  came the “by-product,” a terministic link, Deacon and
  Cashman make, back to the orthodox schema, a reader would
  presume.
  
      Another hint of a dislogistic sort in
  this second section on the symbolic “bi-layered world”
  is how Deacon, along with Cashman, handle the term
  “symbolic savant.”  I made reference to this term
  in a previous post.  Deacon employes it in another of
  his articles on the “Symbolic Species.”  There, in
  contrast, the expression stands alone in its grandeur. 
  Here, “symbolic savant” is explicitly joined at the
 hip
  with the analogous term it so readily brings to mind:
  “idiot savant.”  “Idiot savants” can wondrously
  perform cognitive feats that flabbergast the rest of us,
  like solve complex math problems in an instant.  But in
  most other arenas of life, they’re not so proficient.
  
      Add to these dribs and drabs of cold
  water the authors may be splashing on religion two more
  points: Deacon and Cashman conclude here that the
 religious
  sensibility they describe is altogether “natural” in
  origin.  Nothing “supernatural” is involved. 
  That’s surely what we would expect.  Deacon adds in
  his second Ginn Lecture, though, that transcendentalizing
  “teleology” is “redundant.”  Exactly what
  Deacon means by that isn’t clear.  But its
  implication is.
  
      This intro to the second division of
  “Role of Symbolic Capacity in the Origins of Religion”
  took longer than I expected.  I’ll get to the details
  of the section part of the authors’ argument in the next
  post.
  
  
      Ed
  
  
      P.S. I forgot to mention, in this section
  of “Origins of Religion,” Deacon and Cashman add
  “music” as part and parcel of this “by-product”
  scenario.  “Music,” the authors say, is a
  “semiotic vehicle able to transport us into a world of
  fluid half emotions lying behind and evoked by the
  sounds.”  Interestingly, mega-orthodox evolutionists
  Stephen Jay Gould and Richard Lewontin add female orgasms
 to
  religion and music as “nonadaptive byproducts” of the
  evolutionary process (“The Spandrels of San Marco and
 the
  Panglossian Paradigm: A Critique of the Adaptionist
  Programme,” 1979).  I’ve always said on this
  matter, check first with a paid soprano in a church
  choir.             
       
  
  --------------------------------------------
  On Fri, 3/13/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
  wrote:
  
   Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
   To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
   Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
  <kb at kbjournal.org>
   Date: Friday, March 13, 2015, 4:36 PM
   
   Burkophiles,
   
       On the “synergy” that generates
   narrative, the first of the three ways evolved
 symbolic
   capacity transformed the pre-human mammalian brain
 into
  the
   powerful force for “action,” and theotropic
  speculation
   and motivation, it is today, according to Deacon and
   Cashman:
   
       All mammals (maybe birds, as well),
   Deacon and Cashman say, possess two mnemonic systems,
 two
   means of generating the memory that makes for
 enhanced
   adaptation to an environment and shifting
   circumstances.  One mnemonic agency is
   “procedural.”  The other is “episodic.”  D
   and C label the procedural, seemingly the
 lower-level,
   apparatus for retaining memory traces,
   “diachronic.”  The diachronic is concerned with,
 or
   pertains to, the historical DEVELOPMENT of
 something. 
   It is not of a uniform age or time.  Hence,
 diachronic
   or procedural memory fixes in the mammalian brain
 the
  means
   for rote motor skills, habits, eventually automatic
   responses that become “second nature,” as
 repeated
  again
   and again,  Procedural memory particularly enlists
 the
   basal ganglia, the motor cortex, and the
 cerebellum. 
   (These would appear to be lower-order
   neuro-mechanisms.  Yet, Deacon has said to me in
   correspondence that he does not hold to any strict
 notion
    of the tripartite brain.  This stance would seem to
   suggest that the brain works as something of a whole,
 an
   integrated unit of a sort.  So, I would need
 further
   clarification from Deacon on distinctions between
   “higher” and “lower” neuro-operations, a
  distinction
   he and Cashman seem to be making in this article.
   )
       “Episodic” memory, which I will call
   here a higher-order mnemonic system (after D and C,
 note
   below) is “synchronic,” Deacon and Cashman
 assert. 
   Episodic memory pertains to “individual events and
   relationships.”  It has to do with the state of
   something at one particular time.  Though “classes
 of
   episodic memories” can be retained, the
 “different
   [neuro] structures” involved make for a
 “different
  kind
   of redundancy” in respect to the episodic. 
 “Serial
   redundancy is unavailable.”  The hippocampus is
   integral to the “distributive” nature of
 episodic
   retention.  “The hippocampus is reciprocally
   connected with many different areas of generalized
   neocortex.”  Nested in the cerebrum, it
 “correlates
   converging information from higher-order processing
 in
  each
   sensory modality.”  Episodic “redundancy” is
   found “in context to other events to which it was
   linked.”
   
       Now, as already indicated, all mammals,
   maybe birds, also, benefit from both kinds memory,
 the
   procedural and the episodic.  In those nonsymbolic
   animals, however, the two memory systems are
 separated,
  not
   integrated.  The symbolic capacity that the
 emerging
   species homo sapiens evolved into across the
  2-million-year
   Quaternary brought the two mnemonic systems into a
   revolutionary synergy.  It is symbols that
 integrated
   and integrate the two types of memory.  The
 following
   is the result:  This is how it is done:
   
   “        Syntax,” word order,
   “becomes part of our procedural memory
 system.” 
   “When we produce a sentence, it is a bit like
 riding a
   bike . . . . It is proceduralized.  But what we are
   doing with it is accessing and ‘downloarding’
   information from episodic memory in order to express
 an
  idea
   or accomplish a communicative action.”
   
           Thus, “the interplay between
   the serial [that is, the procedural] and
 distributive
   associative [i.e., the episodic] tendencies brought
 into
   interaction by language provides a way to organize
  episodic
   memory into sequences.”  And so, “the narrative
   predisposition [that results] can be understood as
 an
   emergent consequence of the unique mnemonic synergy
 that
   language has made possible.”
   So aver Deacon and Cashman.
   
           Now, what is distinctive about
   the ensuing “narrative” that nudges symbolizers
 toward
   religion, or metaphysical speculations?  Narrative
 is
   not merely the equivalent of the proverbial put-down
 of
   “history” as simply “one darned thing after
   another.”  Narrative is not just stories about
   synchronic episodes that progress according to
 diachronic
   habitual sequences.  Narratives tend to be
 peculiarly
   “directed” sequences, explanations,
 interpretations,
   allegations, justifications, commentaries, personal
 or
  group
   representations of the facts of the matter, made via
  causal
   connections, with indictment or praise for the
 parties
   involved (Deacon & Cashman; Shorter OED). 
 Indeed,
   narratives can generate “a sense of present or
 potential
   loss” that fuses “present, past, and imagined
   experiences” (Deacon & Cashman) in a way that
  conjures
   Neo-Freudian Norman O. Brown’s dramatistic
 explanation
  of
   how human narration creates “time.”
     In the “id” there is no time, says Freud. 
   In the ego, however, “Time has to be constructed by
 an
   animal that has guilt [or a sense of loss] and seeks
 to
   expiate [or redeem or correct that sense of
 loss].” 
   Such a being must create and dwell on the notions of
 a
   “past” and a “future.”  “Archaic man
 [sic;
   and modern man and woman] experiences guilt and
 therefore
   time” (Brown, Life Against Death: The
 Psychoanalytical
   Meaning of History, 1959, pp. 274, 276, 278,
 87-109).`
   
           Even more to the point than the
   inherently dramatic nature of narrative plot lines
 and
   narrative’s temporal fusion of “loss” with a
 vision
  of
   something redemptively better, is the stark contrast
  between
   the “telos” essential to the narrative impulse,
 and
  the
   lack of such consummation in many, can we not say
 all,
  human
   lives.  The sense of narrative near impels
 symbolizers,
   Deacon and Cashman contend, toward transcendental
 plot
   lines.  “They [narratives] do not simply stop
   arbitrarily, as do most lives,” Deacon and Cashman
   observe.  “Instead, in the narrative of a life,
   whether real or imagined, birth and death are events
 that
   are usually subordinated to some telos, . . . .” 
 The
   gross narrative of a human life as actually lived,
  abridged
   of any larger meaning in itself, tends to be
  “embedded”
   in, or related to, a larger narrative that
 overmatches the
   glaring limitations of the “brief candle” of
 human
   existence.
   
           “Telos”: “End, purpose,
   (an) ultimate object or aim” (Shorter OED).
   
           Later, two more “synergies,”
   generated by language, that might well help to
 explain
  still
   further the otherwise strange otherworldliness of
 the
   symbol-using animal.
   
   
           Ed       
                  
          
          
   
   --------------------------------------------
   On Thu, 3/12/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
   wrote:
   
    Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
    To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
    Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
   <kb at kbjournal.org>
    Date: Thursday, March 12, 2015, 3:53 PM
    
    Burkophiles,
    
        At the outset of their article, “The
    Role of Symbolic Capacity in the Origins of
  Religion”
    (2009), Terrence W. Deacon and his coauthor Tyrone
  Cashman
    cite polling data that 86 percent of human beings
  believe
   in
    God or some sort of Supreme Being.  Burke, as
  I’ve
    argued, gives good reasons why that is the case
    IMPLICITLY.  From a very different academic vantage
    point, Deacon and Cashman do so EXPLICITLY. 
  Language
    is central in the case they, the three of them, make
  for
   the
    ubiquity of belief in the god-term, and the case for
  the
    global occurrence of belief in the Divine, as
   characterized
    in many variant ways.
    
        Deacon, the biological
    anthropologist/neuroscientist (I know nothing about
   Cashman)
    is coming at the relationship between symbolic
  capacity
   and
    the religious “predisposition” very much from an
    evolutionary and neuroscientific angle.  One
  notable
    point needs made at the outset: Forget about homo
    sapiens/homo dialecticus, language in tow, suddenly
    appearing on the scene circa 200,000 years ago.  D
  and
    C say language development and brain size-and-
  structure
    development gradually and reciprocally occurred over
  the
    entire Pleistocene, which is to say, across the
  whole
    near-2-million-year duration of the Quaternary
  Period. 
    Brain evolution gradually brought linguistic facility
  into
    being, while, at the same time, emerging symbolic
  capacity
    reciprocally altered the human brain.
    
        (Sounds a bit analogous to the
    Wallace/Lovelock/Gaia hypothesis on reciprocal
  changes in
    both life and the nonliving material scene over 3.5
   billion
    years, but that’s another matter.)
    
        At the outset, also, the coauthors
    summarize the three most-cited theories about the
  origin
   of
    religion via evolutionary change: religion as
    “nonadaptive,” or useless, by-product of
  formerly
    adaptive changes, “misapplications,” so to speak,
  of
    evolved tendencies; more or less “adaptive”
  proofs
    against mortality; and “parasitic memes”
  concocted
    socially, rather than individually, as oppressive
   sanctions
    of one kind or another.  The first of the above is
  the
    most favored.  All these explanations are
  inadequate
    “reductionistic” takes on this universal human
    phenomenon, Deacon and Cashman contend.  
    To get to the pith and marrow of the issue, as Deacon
  and
    Cashman see it: With the evolution of language,
  three
    “synergies” emerged that made religious
  speculation
   and
    belief, as well as the general “metaphysical”
  search
   for
    underlying “First Principles,” an inevitable
   consequence
    of biological change  (“Metaphysics” for
    Burke?  “Coy theology,” you will recall).
    
        “Synergy”: “The production by two
    or more agents, substances [structures, capacities,
  etc.]
   of
    a combined effect greater than the sum or their
  separate
    effects . . . Increased effectiveness or achievement
    produced by combined action.”
    
        “Emergent,” quite similar in meaning:
    “An effect produced by a combination of causes but
   unable
    to be seen as the sum of their individual effects”
    (Shorter OED).
    
        Three “synergies,” occasioned by the
    evolution of symbol use over that long span of time,
   brought
    into being this “religious” or
  “metaphysical”
   being,
    the “symbolic species,” making religion, D and C
   assert,
    humankind’s “synergy of synergies.”  These
    combinations of, or symbol-generated interactions
  between,
    structures and capacities our mammalian ancestors
   possessed
    and utilized, are: 
    (1)    “The role of language in a novel
    synergy between 2 previously orthogonal modes of
  memory
    storage which is the basis for the narrative
   predisposition
    that is distinctively characteristic of human
  reasoning,
    identity, and culture.”
    
    (2)    A tropism toward conception of what D
    and C call a “bi-layered world,” namely, an
  “evolved
    attentional bias toward discerning a pattern
    behind-the-pattern, a bias required for language
  learning,
    which makes metaphysical dualism intuitively natural,
  and
    also makes the double-world metaphysics common to
  most
    religions a likely leap of symbolic imagination.”
    
    (3)     “The dramatic expansion
    and transformation of the mammalian emotional
  repertoire
   by
    virtue of the use of symbolic blends to induce
   unprecedented
    interactions and novel experiential synergies that
  we
    describe as emergent emotional experiences.”
    
    “Orthogonal”: As inferred from Deacon’s work as
  a
    whole, the meaning here, I would assume, is
  “straight,
    normal, proper [I think we can add “natural”],
  without
    external influence,” “completely independent,”
  which
    is to say, “free” of influence by the
  “absential
    feature,” or nonsymbolic negative, that nonhuman
  living
    beings are beholden to.  Hence, we’re talking
  here
    about the  “orthograde.”  “Orthograde,” in
    Deaconese, serves, it seems to me, as the opposite
  of
    “contragrade.”  “Contragrade” refers to a
  force
    that runs counter to mechanical nonliving processes.
     
        Yet, there seems to me to be an ambiguity
    here with this term.  “Orthogonal,” seen as a
    derivative of “orthograde,” would likely
  characterize
    nonsymbolic living beings, as well as symbolic. 
  Lower
    animals and plants appear to be “contragrade,”
  too, in
    the sense that they uniquely resist entropy by way
  of
    reproduction and photosynthesis, or by reproduction
  and
    direct or indirect ingestion of the products of
    photosynthesis.  Maybe I can get Deacon to bring me
  up
    to speed on the definition “orthogonal,” as
  employed
    here, when I forward the post to him.
    
        I’ll try to unpack the three synergies
    in subsequent posts.
    
    
        Ed      
                   
        
    
    --------------------------------------------
    On Fri, 3/6/15, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
    wrote:
    
     Subject: Re: [KB] Burke, Deacon, and Theology
     To: "Clarke Rountree" <rountrj at uah.edu>
     Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
    <kb at kbjournal.org>
     Date: Friday, March 6, 2015, 8:57 PM
     
     Clarke,
     
     Thanks for your extended and
     insightful reply to my post.  Hey, thanks for
 even
    reading
     my long rant!  You certainly make some good
 points,
    though
     I would interpret the "potentiality" claim in RM
     as making our emergence out of a "wordless"
 Ground
     as maybe "more complicated" than that. 
 Following
     Burke, I could quote Scripture here, a passage in
   Romans
    and
     one in Hebrews on how the visible things of this
   world
     adumbrate a reality that is beyond.  I'll let
   Deacon
     expand on the matter in his own way in a
 subsequent
   post.
     
     (Not that Deacon is
     necessarily a theist.  The religious sensibility
   that
    comes
     so "naturally" to the "symbolic species"
     is, for Deacon, just that: inevitable, yes, but
   natural. 
     "Teleology" is real, one of the last frames in
 his
     second Ginn Lecture proclaims, but to
   transcendentalize it
     is "redundant, Deacon avers.  An unsatisfactory
     denouement for the "symbol-users" or the
     "symbolic species," i.e., coming at the matter
     from either Burke's perspective, or Deacon's. 
     That's why 86 percent of Americans believe in
 God,
   or
     some such figure, as Deacon and Cashman note at
 the
     beginning of their journal article.)
     
     By way of illustration, I offered in one of my
     posts to Deacon the characters in Samuel Becket's
   black
     comedy, "Waiting for Godot."  At the end of the
     play, they are still waiting, and the
 implications
   are
    that
     Godot is not going to show up, yet these forlorn
   wretches
     will continue to wait . . and wait . . . and wait
 . .
   .
    and
     never stop waiting.  
     
     On
     the symbol-user's tendency to "see" drama in,
     or superimpose drama on, the supposedly blind
 motions
   of
    the
     universe---Deacon has a powerful explanation for
   that,
    which
     I'll get to eventually.
     
     Thanks again for replying---and rebutting!
     
     
     
     Ed
     
     
     
     
     
     
     
      
     --------------------------------------------
     On Fri, 3/6/15, Clarke Rountree <rountrj at uah.edu>
     wrote:
     
      Subject: Re: [KB]
     Burke, Deacon, and Theology
      To:
     "Edward C Appel" <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
      Cc: "kb at kbjournal.org"
     <kb at kbjournal.org>
      Date: Friday, March 6, 2015, 12:23 PM
      
      Dear Burkelers--
      My good friend Ed obviously
     
     has a passion for seeing Burke as a theologian
      (coy or otherwise), despite Burke’s
     insistence that he
      isn’t a believer in
     God.
      Of course one can get around the
     problem of Burke not
      actually believing in
     God through the
      Spinozan strategy of making
     God equal to Nature, so that the
      GROUND of
     things
      itself is equivalent to God. And,
     notably, if verbalizing is
      somehow
     contained
      in or implicit in that GROUND,
     then, Ed reasons, that is an
      acknowledgment
     of
      at least a God principle at work. Even
     an old atheist like
      me will admit that
      we humans, surprisingly, arose from a wordless
     world, that
      post facto we know carried the
     potentiality for
      creating wordy
      creature. Note, also, that Burke insists that
     when humans
      are gone it will go
      back to its wordlessness. (I’m speaking of
     Earth only, of
      course; I think it
      perfectly obvious that there is sentient life
     elsewhere in
      the universe.)
      
      
      Last year
     my son John and
      I published our essay from
     the
      Burke conference in Belgium where we
     argued about the
      potentialities and
      dangers of the motivational bias of humans.
     (We called it a
      symbol-users
      guide. I don't have the citation handy,
     but I can get it
      if anyone's
     interested.) That is, building on my
     
     “Dramatism as Literal” essay in KBJ (2010),
 we
      noted that it is intrinsic to humans to see
     the world
      through the grammar of
      motives, and that this grammar becomes a
     problematic
      terministic screen. (It
      also is a wonderful thing, of course, as it
     makes us
      recognizably human—my point
      in the 2010 essay.) After mulling over that
     issue, I have a
      new appreciation
      for Burke’s suggestion that toddlers may
     learn the idea of
      “No” before they
      learn the idea of “nothing.” The
     implication is that
      “scientific” or
     “objective”
      understandings of the world
     are an add-on—something
      secondary we have
     to learn,
      while the search for motives is
     primary.
      
      
      Now, early humans learned
      to
     search for motives in prey and
      in animals
     that preyed on them. Scientists may tell us now
      that animals don’t
      have the
     capacity for the kind of actions we may attribute
      to them, but
      thinking that
     way may have been beneficial in making us wary
      of what animals
      are doing
     when they run or attack. Of course, walking
 around
      with a “pentadic”
      set of
     glasses on the world undoubtedly gives rise to
      animism and, later, more
     
     complex forms of religion, with gods in the
 river,
   the
     sea,
      the sky, etc.
      
       
      
      (Let me
     add quickly as
      well, that the dramatistic
     screen did not handicap humans
      in
     developing technology and improving their lot;
   it’s
      just that every agency
      was
     connected to a human purpose, every scene
 implicitly
      asked “what can be
      done
     [for humans] with this?” Indeed, that's one of
      the warning John and I make--that we have
     trouble seeing
      anything except as it
     relates to us.)
      
      
      When scientists look at
      the
     world as objects, they blind
      themselves to
     this primary way of thinking (though, as Burke
      says, not so much that
      they
     don’t know to treat their fellow chemists
   differently
      than the chemicals
      with which
     they work). When people want to look at more than
      things,
      investigating the
     human and the social, wearing these
     
     blinders threaten to make
      them miss the
     FUNCTIONS of pentadic screens in human
     
     interactions.
      
      
      Now Burke, looking at
     
     religion, would be missing much if he
      were
     so blinded; but of course he is not. He not only
      accounts for the key term
     
     ACTION in human relations (while excoriating
 those
   who
     focus
      on MOTION), but he
     
     investigates the potentialities of symbolic
 action.
   That
      this leads him to find
      a
     particularly potent (and “perfected”)
 symbolic
   form
     in
      religion is
     
     unsurprising—religion is one of the oldest,
 most
      scrutinized, most pervasive,
     
     most defended, most argued over, and (thus) most
     perfected
      symbolic systems we
      have. 
      
      
      Ultimately, I believe that
     
     the perfectedness of religion as
      a symbol
     system has nothing to do with its intrinsic truth
      (except as, perhaps, an "end of the
     line" human
      truth, an implication of
     our terminology). It does have much
      to do
     with its relation to power, making it a key bone
 of
      contention for those
      who
     would rise to prominence in social systems. And
      Burke’s focus on religion
     
     has more to do with the fact that, given its
 intense
      scrutiny by some of the
     
     brightest minds for millennia (at least since
   Augustine)
     it
      is the most thoroughgoing
      symbolic system around. However, as I noted in
     a QJS
      article on the construction of George
     H.W. Bush in the
      1992
     
     presidential elections (1995), there are other
 well
      developed symbol systems that have
      pushed the envelope of what is “thinkable”
     through our
      dramatistic grammar—my key
      example, criminal law, where hundreds of years
     of
      Anglo-American law (and
     
     earlier law as well) helped to refine the
   possibilities
     for
      guilt and
      innocence.
     Had Burke spent more time talking to law folks,
      maybe he would have
      landed on
     criminal law as a perfection of symbol systems
      (though, note, he does
      spend
     a lot of time with constitutions!). That
 wouldn’t
      make him a lawyer; and
     
     finding perfection in religion doesn’t make him
 a
      theologian. Just an admirer
     
     of what has been wrought.
      
     
     
      That’s my oar in the
     
     water.
      
      
     
     (On a side note, I’m a
      great fan of
     Ed’s explication of
      Deacon’s work and
     its implications for understanding
     
     Burke’s work. I look forward to the next
     
     installment.)
      Cheers,
     
     Clarke
      On Thu, Mar 5, 2015 at 2:52
      PM, Edward C Appel <edwardcappel at frontier.com>
      wrote:
      Burkophiles,
      
      
      
              I’ve posted at length here on
     intersections
      between Burke and Terrence W.
     Deacon, Chair of the
      Department of
     Anthropology at Cal Berkeley, you may
     
     recall.  (Of equal importance, Deacon is a
     neuroscientist,
      as well.)  In response to
     a recent e-mail of mine on his
      aptly
     described “tour de force,” Incomplete Nature:
   How
      Mind Emerged from Matter (Norton, 2012, 2013),
     Professor
      Deacon kindly sent me additional
     materials of his: two more
      of his published
     articles, bringing my cache to seven in
     
     toto; and several series of exceedingly well
 wrought
      powerpoint frames and other visuals he has
     used in lectures
      at the University of Oslo
     in Norway, in Holland, the Ginn
      Lectures in
     Atlanta, and a couple of presentations to
     
     theologians.
      
      
      
              As some of you
     will likely suspect, the theology
      theme
     immediately piqued my interest.  Here we have,
 it
      seemed to me, another important point of
     convergence between
      Burke and Deacon,
     adding to the considerable list I’ve
     
     already outlined.   The focal reference here
 will
   be to
      Deacon’s coauthored article (with Tyrone
     Cashman), “The
      Role of Symbolic Capacity
     in the Origins of Religion,”
      Journal of
     Religion, Nature & Culture, 2009, Vol. 3 No.
      4, pp. 490-517.
      
      
      
           
       I want to begin, in this post, with my take on
      Burke’s anfractuous relationship to
     theology, then in a
      subsequent edition,
     summarize Deacon’s position.
      
      
      
           
       Burke, we know, claimed publicly that his
      interest in theology was entirely secular. 
     “Logology,”
      the late-Burke title for
     his philosophy looked at as an
     
     “epistemology,” was solely about the contours
 of
      symbolic action, its motives and tendentious
     operations, not
      about any putatively
     transcendental reality.  Logology was
      the
     “systematic study of theological terms for the
   light
      they might throw on the forms of language,”
     theological
      terms being the most
     thoroughgoing, far-reaching, ultimate
      terms
     in language,” language, in Burke’s pat
 phrase,
      taken to “the end of the line.”  Tim
     Crusius doubled
      down on Burke’s
     affirmation in Kenneth Burke and the
     
     Conversation After Philosophy (Southern Illinois
 UP,
     1999),
      and in his review of Greig
     Henderson’s book,  Kenneth
      Burke:
     Literature and Language as Symbolic Action, in
 QJS
   76
      (1990), pp. 340-342.
      
      
      
           
       Add to Burke’s official position on logology
      (was it, or was it not, something of a façade
     that a
      cluster/agon analysis can maneuver
     around?---I ask, and have
      asked) Burke’s
     private claim to have been a nontheist. 
     
     Wayne Booth  (“Wax ‘N Wayne,” as Burke
 would
   call
     him
      privately) was a frequent correspondent
     of KB’s.  In his
      chapter, “Kenneth
     Burke’s Religious Rhetoric:
     
     ‘God-Terms’ and the Ontological Proof,” in
     Rhetorical
      Invention and Religious Inquiry
     (Yale UP, 2000, pp. 25-46),
      Booth takes
     note of Burke’s demurrer.  (See, also, “The
      Many Voices of Kenneth Burke, Theologian and
     Prophet, as
      Revealed in His Letters to
     Me,” in Unending Conversations:
      New
     Writings by and about Kenneth Burke, Eds.
 Henderson
   and
      Williams [Southern Illinois UP, 2001, pp.
     179-201], where
      Booth offers a second
     reprise on his 1996 plenary address at
      the
     Duquesne Conference, i.e., the Third Triennial.)
      
      
      
              On his actual belief or nonbelief,
     I think we
      have to take Burke at his
     word.  Let’s assume he was the
      nontheist
     he told people he was.
      
     
     
      
              That having
     been said, Burke’s demonstrable
     
     theological obsession cannot be gainsaid.  In
   Literature
      and Language (1988), Henderson looked behind
     the curtain:
      “For Burke logology is in
     some sense a surrogate
      theology.”  Greig
     added, “The analogies he makes for
     
     heuristic purposes betray a psychological need for
 a
     sense
      of permanence akin to a religious
     faith in the curative
      power of the word
     made flesh” (p. 105).  That same year,
     
     at ECA (April 29), Trevor Melia speculated about
   “what
      kind of Christian” Burke was, suggested that
     the label
      “secular Christian” can only
     be a starting point, and
      concluded that
     Burke was at least “up to his ears in
     
     Christianity.”
      
      
      
              At length, I had
     already corresponded with Burke
      on the
     matter (late 1983, early 1984), my case,
 “Kenneth
      Burke: Coy Theologian,” later published in
     the Journal of
      Communication and Religion
     (September, 1993).  Burke had
      even allowed
     at the Philadelphia Conference (March,1984)
     
     that I had made a “powerful argument” that he
 was
   a
      theologian.  I got that news from Herb
     Simons, who was
      present at the after-hours
     discussion several key scholars
      had had
     with Burke after the first day’s seminars.
      
      
      
              Other names that can be added to
     these
      speculations about Burke and religion
     include, as per Booth
      in that Yale
     publication, Burks, Carter, Duerden, Durham,
      Freccero, Gunn, Gusfield, and Jay.  And I can
     add Richard
      Thames and Steven Mailloux to
     their number (Steve’s paper
      at the Ghent
     Conference: “Under the Sign of Theology:
     
     Kenneth Burke on Language and the Supernatural
      Order.")
      
      
      
           
       I won’t lay out my entire case on Burke as
      “coy theologian.”  But I’ll highlight a
     few central
      points:
      
      
      
           
       The bottom line in my correspondence with KB,
      and in my journal piece, was this: It
     doesn’t matter what
      Burke personally
     believed or did not believe.  When a
     
     theorist posits that symbolizers are inherently
      “theotropic”---I’m borrowing a term from
     Steve
      here---that theorist is at least a
     “generic”
      theologian.  A “lure” in
     rhetoric, whether in
      “error” or not,
     strongly nudges homo loquax “Upward”
     
     not only toward a “god term” in general, but
   also
     toward
      the most “perfectly” satisfying
     God-term of all (GM, pp.
      306; RM, pp.
     275-76, 290-91; RR).
      
      
      
              And especially
     when it can be related to extant
      or
     historic theological systems, that generic
 theology
   takes
      on some kind of shape, affords a bit of
     implicit commentary
      this ubiquitous
     attribute.  I’ve characterized Burke’s
     
     dramatism/logology as a quasi-gnostic (a radical
   sense of
     a
      “Fall” into language, not into a
     lustful body)
      universalism (the quest for a
     “god-term” that unites all
      of humanity,
     and a focal program aimed at “purifying”
      conflict and “war”), friendly to
     Whitehead’s process
      theology (with its
     dialectics, de-perfecting of the Godhead,
     
     and rejection of a life after death; note what
 Burke
   said
     in
      the movie shown at Airlie House,
     1993).
      
      
     
     
              Add to these pillars of
     support what I would
      conceive as Burke’s
     occasional drift into theological
     
     principles in the paradigm sense, i.e.,
 statements
   that
     have
      to do with, or surely adumbrate, the
     actual existence of a
      Divine Essence of a
     kind.  When Burke says the
      “extrahuman
     ground” out of which humans proceeded
     
     “contains the principle of personality, quite
 as
   it
      contains the principle of verbalizing,” that
     this
      “’nonverbal’ ground must have
     contained the
      ‘potentiality’ of the
     verbal, otherwise the verbal could
      not have
     emerged from it,” Burke has crossed the line, I
      would suggest, into theology or religion
     proper (RM, pp.
      289-90).
     
     
      
      
         
         An analogous proposition is found in the
      Calabi-Yau version of string theory via the
     “anthropic
      principle,” the “notion
     that the observed laws of nature
      must be
     consistent with the presence of intelligent life
      and, specifically, the presence of intelligent
     observers
      like us.  Put in other terms,
     the universe looks the way it
      does because
     if conditions were even slightly different,
     
     life would not have formed and humans would not
 be
   around
     to
      observe it” (Shing-Tung Yau and Steve
     Nadis, The Shape of
      Inner Space: String
     Theory and the Geometry of the
      Universe’s
     Hidden Dimensions , Basic Books, 2010, p.
     
     345).
      
      
     
     
              Analogous, also, as I see it,
     is this quotation
      highlighted in one of
     Deacon’s two Ginn Lectures: “We
      need an
     understanding of nature such that it is not
 absurd
      to say that it has us as its products,” by
     Belgian chemist
      Ilya Prigogine and the
     physicist Victor J. Stenger. 
     
     (“Naturalizing Teleology: The Redemption of
 Science
   by
     the
      Rediscovery of Self and Value,”
     Deacon, 2014.)
      
      
      
              Illustrative of
     what different observers will
      “see” in
     a given statement, question, lacuna, or
     
     phenomenon, Stenger was one of the “new
   atheists,”
      author of God: The Failed Hypothesis: How
     Science Shows That
      God Does not Exist,
     2007.  Stenger, former blogger for the
     
     Huffington Post, famously said, “Science flies
 you
   to
     the
      moon.  Religion flies you into
     buildings.”  One could
      just as
     “truthfully” say, “Religion prepares meals,
      low cost or free, for the elderly and needy in
     Lancaster
      Country, Pennsylvania, and in
     other communities across the
      world.  The
     likes of godless communism and Nazism kills a
      hundred million persons, give or take a few
     tens of
      millions, in quest of a
     ‘heaven’ on earth.”
      
     
     
      
              As Burke
     says, symbols unite and divide, select
      in
     and select out, induce attention toward and
 induce
      attention away from.  Pick the blinkered
     lenses of your
      choice.  They’re all
     free!
      
      
     
     
              So, later, an examination of
     the case for the
      ubiquity of religion
     Deacon and Cashman make in the article
      that
     concludes: “We speculate that something like a
      religious predisposition, in the most general
     sense of the
      term, should be considered a
     universal consequence of the
      symbolic
     capacity evolving, whether here on earth, or in
 any
      other context where symbolic cognition might
     arise.”
      
      
      
      
      
              Ed
      
      
      
      
      
     
     _______________________________________________
      
      KB mailing list
      
      KB at kbjournal.org
      
      http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
      
      
      
      
      -- 
      Dr.
      Clarke Rountree
      Chair and
     Professor of
      Communication Arts
      342 Morton Hall
      University of
     Alabama in Huntsville
      Huntsville, AL 
     35899
      256-824-6646
      clarke.rountree at uah.edu
      
     
     _______________________________________________
     KB mailing list
     KB at kbjournal.org
     http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
     
    
    _______________________________________________
    KB mailing list
    KB at kbjournal.org
    http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
    
   
   _______________________________________________
   KB mailing list
   KB at kbjournal.org
   http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
   
  
  _______________________________________________
  KB mailing list
  KB at kbjournal.org
  http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
  
 
 _______________________________________________
 KB mailing list
 KB at kbjournal.org
 http://kbjournal.org/mailman/listinfo/kb_kbjournal.org
 




More information about the KB mailing list